As a learner eager to expand your knowledge, whether it is mastering a new language, acing an exam, or deepening your understanding of complex subjects you may have come across SRS tools like Anki. However, while SRS can be a powerful ally, several pitfalls can trip you up. This is even more true, if you're used to the structured flow of pre-made courses. Let’s explore these challenges so you can understand why they happen and set yourself up for long-term success by setting strategies to overcome them or even prevent them.
Pitfall 1
There is an increasing overload of flashcards pending to be reviewed (scheduled for today or in the past) (1,2).
The Overload Dilemma: When Reviews Overwhelm Your Day
Over time, a user’s daily review load becomes larger than what is realistically manageable. This overload can lead to stress (X), decreased motivation (X), and ultimately, ineffective learning, as the user struggles to keep up with the accumulating material.
Why It Happens:
Excessive Content Generation: Many users start with pre-made decks or AI-generated flashcards that include more cards than they realistically have time to review daily. This creates an initial situation where the potential review load is already high and very quickly will exceed their daily capacity.
Inconsistent Use: Skipping days causes a buildup of overdue reviews (1). When reviews aren’t done consistently, the spaced repetition system’s schedule gets thrown off, resulting in an ever-increasing pile of overdue cards.
Strategies to Overcome This Pitfall:
Remember, disruption doesn’t have to be complicated. By breaking down the problem into small, achievable steps, you can make significant progress without feeling overwhelmed. Small changes, when repeated consistently, lead to big improvements over time.
Set Realistic Daily Goals: Limit your review count to a manageable number to avoid burnout, apps like Anki usually have a dedicated setting for this.
Prioritize High-Yield Cards: Identify which cards are most critical (for example, those covering core vocabulary or essential concepts) and review those first.
Build Consistency: Even short, daily sessions can prevent an overwhelming backlog.
Pitfall 2
Most of the benefits of spaced repetition are seen long term; new users must invest time and effort before reaping positive results (3–6).
Delayed Gratification: Most Benefits appear in the long run
Spaced repetition follows the same pattern as weight loss. At first, it feels like nothing is happening: you struggle to recall information, and it might feel discouraging. But just like with weight loss, progress is happening beneath the surface: your brain is strengthening neural connections, even if you don’t "see" it yet.
Following the analogy, when someone starts a weight loss journey, they often expect to see immediate results. Perhaps dropping a few pounds in the first week but after that initial drop, progress slows down. The scale might not change for days or even weeks, leading to frustration. Many people quit at this stage because they believe their efforts aren’t working.
However, real fat loss is happening beneath the surface. The body is adjusting, metabolism is adapting, and long-term consistency is what leads to lasting transformation. Those who stick with it eventually see a breakthrough—suddenly, their clothes fit better, energy levels rise, and the results become obvious.
If you quit SRS too early, it’s like abandoning a weight loss plan just because the scale didn’t move for a week. The key to success in both is trusting the process and staying consistent, even when progress feels invisible. Over time, retrieval becomes effortless, just like maintaining a healthy weight feels natural after long-term habits are established.
Why It Happens:
Slow Onset of Benefits: The spaced repetition process is designed to build durable long-term memory, which naturally takes time (7,8).
Psychological Hurdles: A lack of immediate rewards can diminish motivation (9,10), especially after long, demanding workdays.
Strategies to Overcome This Pitfall:
Embrace a Long-Term Perspective: Recognize that SRS is a marathon, not a sprint. The true rewards of spaced repetition aren’t immediate but they’re definitely worth the wait. It’s easy to get frustrated when you feel like you’re not making fast progress, but spaced repetition thrives on compound gains. What feels slow today will become effortless recall in the future. Think of it like planting seeds: you won’t see results overnight, but with consistent effort, the harvest will come.
Monitor Small Wins: Keep track of incremental improvements—even small gains add up. When reviewing, I like to remind myself that every card I recall correctly is a tiny victory but is not lost. Each victory is small but adds up! A small process that is never undone.
Reward Consistency: Establish milestones to celebrate progress and keep your motivation high. Instead of focusing on "perfect" days, I aim for consistency streaks. Even if I just review a few cards (there were more scheduled), I count it as a win.
Pitfall 3
The representation of knowledge is a limiting factor for memory retention (11–14).
The Art of Knowledge Representation
How you encode information plays a critical role in whether you remember it. Poorly formulated flashcards, which rely on vague prompts or passive recognition cues, can lead to weak memory traces that fade quickly.
Example 1
Q:What are the mitochondria?
A:The powerhouse of the cell.
Why it's bad?
The question is too open-ended and doesn't guide recall effectively.
The answer is vague and doesn't encourage deeper understanding.
✅ Better Version:
Q:What is the function of mitochondria in energy production?
B:Mitochondria generate ATP through cellular respiration, acting as the cell’s main energy provider.
Example 2
Q:"The _______ is known as the powerhouse of the cell because it generates ATP through cellular respiration."
A) Nucleus
B) Golgi apparatus
C) Mitochondrion
D) Ribosome
A: C
Why it's bad?
Encourages Recognition Instead of Active Recall [glossary term]. Recognition is easier than recall, but recall leads to stronger memory traces and longer retention.
Learners often recognize the correct answer rather than recalling it from memory
✅ Better Version:
Q: Which organelle is the powerhouse of the cell?”
Skill Development: How you frame questions for reading comprehension or note-taking is a skill in itself, but formulating Q&A pairs that trigger atomic memories is an even more advanced one. Designing effective flashcards isn’t just about storing information, it’s about shaping it in a way that strengthens recall and understanding. As you progress, you’ll refine your cards between repetitions, either adjusting wording or reworking concepts to solidify your knowledge. This flexibility allows for continuous learning, making each review an opportunity to deepen understanding rather than just reinforce recall.
Strategies to Overcome This Pitfall:
Refine Your Flashcards: Even if you lean on pre-made content, spend a few minutes customizing cards to fit your understanding.
Focus on Active Recall: Design flashcards that prompt you to actively retrieve information rather than simply recognize it.
Learn the Craft: Invest time in understanding best practices for flashcard creation. articles on effective learning (e.g., Wozniak’s “20 Rules of Formulating Knowledge” (15)) offer practical guidance.
Pitfall 4
New users often create flashcards that prompt recognition rather than active recall.
Recognition vs. Active Recall: Avoiding Passive Learning
A common mistake is designing cards where the key term appears as the question, leading to a reliance on recognition rather than testing your ability to retrieve the answer independently (11).
Let’s follow the example of the mitochondria. If we ask what the mitochondria are, we are just recognizing the word, as it is already provided in the question and doesn’t ensure we have an understanding of its function. Each time we get asked this question, we could answer differently, mentioning some details or some others. That is why you will need to do some other flashcards to recall specific functions of the mitochondria. Here are some examples:
Q: What molecule is primarily produced by mitochondria for cellular energy?
A: ATP (adenosine triphosphate).
Q: Which process in mitochondria is directly responsible for ATP production?
A: Electron transport chain (ETC)
Q: What process links the electron transport chain to ATP production?
A: Chemiosmosis
Why It Matters:
Strength of Active Recall: Actively retrieving information reinforces memory more powerfully than mere recognition (11).
Engagement: Active recall engages your cognitive processes more deeply, ensuring that learning is more robust (5).
Long-Term Retention: The process of effortfully retrieving answers improves your long-term retention of the material hence slowing the forgetting process (5).
Strategies to Overcome This Pitfall:
Shifting from passive recognition to active recall not only helps with understanding the material better but also strengthens my memory. Adopting these strategies can make your learning process more effective and rewarding.
Design for Retrieval: Frame questions so that you’re prompted to generate the answer from your own memory rather than being cued by the wording.
Incorporate Reverse Cards: Optionally use two-way flashcards that challenge you to recall both the question from the answer and vice versa.
Vary Your Cues: Create multiple questions for the same concept with different viewpoints to strengthen different retrieval routes. For example:
Q: What organelle is known as the powerhouse of the cell? A: Mitochondria
Q: Which cellular structure is responsible for ATP production? A: Mitochondria
Pitfall 5
Irregular use of SRS leads to a mix of spaced and massed practice, which undermines long-term learning.
Inconsistency in Practice: The Perils of Irregular Review
I used to have a chaotic study schedule. Some days, I’d stick to my reviews religiously, and other days, life got in the way. Soon, I found myself with overdue flashcards piling up, and I attempted to catch up by cramming. This approach felt overwhelming and, ultimately, diminished the long-term benefits of spaced repetition.
Why It Happens:
Busy Schedules: Irregular study routines result in long gaps between reviews.
Psychological Resistance: Early frustrations may cause you to skip days, further exacerbating the issue.
Catching Up: When you try to mass review overdue reviews, you shift from the gentle rhythm of spaced practice to the stress of massed practice. At that point, you have two choices: either slow down the introduction of new flashcards so the load becomes manageable or navigate the constant overflow—a strategy best reserved for advanced users.
Strategies to Overcome This Pitfall:
Establish a Routine: Set aside dedicated time each day for your SRS reviews, even if it is just 5 minutes.
Use Reminders: Leverage app notifications or calendar alerts to keep you on track.
Plan for Interruptions: If you know you’ll miss a day, adjust your settings (e.g., reduce the number of new cards) to ease back into your routine.
Pitfall 6
There’s a temptation to create flashcards for isolated facts that don’t connect with other knowledge, leading to an overload of random trivia.
Random Fact Pitfall: Avoiding Unconnected Flashcards
Filling your deck with unrelated bits of information can clutter your study sessions and dilute the overall effectiveness of your learning, making it harder to see the larger picture. Using flashcards to learn isolated facts often results in a more compartmentalized approach to studying. Interleaved practice [glossary term] tends to promote a more interconnected and flexible learning framework, whereas isolated flashcard learning might limit you to knowing fragments without understanding how they interrelate.
Why It Matters:
Contextual Learning: Memory is enhanced when new information is linked to existing knowledge (16).
Efficiency: Building a network of interconnected concepts is far more effective than memorizing isolated facts (17–19).
Practical Application: For language learning (and other fields), knowing words or concepts in context is key to genuine fluency and understanding (20,21).
Strategies to Overcome This Pitfall:
Link New Cards: Whenever possible, connect new flashcards with previously learned material.
Quality over Quantity: Focus on creating or curating cards that contribute to a coherent body of knowledge.
Integrate Mnemonics: Use mnemonic techniques to bridge the gap between unconnected facts. Example of three unconnected facts:
Fact 1: The mitochondria are the powerhouse of the cell.
Fact 2: The Nile River flows through Egypt.
Fact 3: Alexander the Great was a famous conqueror.
Mnemonic: "The cell’s spark, the Nile’s mark, and Alexander’s lark." While this may sound amusing, creating a rhyme can force you to think of each fact together, making the recall process smoother.
Example A: "In the cell, the powerhouse burns, Like the Nile through Egypt turns, As Alexander's conquest churns."
Example B: "In the cell, the powerhouse glows bright, Like the Nile that flows in endless flight, Where Alexander’s might took its fight."
This version keeps the mnemonic concise while maintaining a strong rhythm and logical flow between the facts. It also makes it easier to remember by reinforcing key associations:
Mitochondria = powerhouse glowing bright
Nile = flowing endlessly
Alexander = rising to great power
Following the example of the mitochondria, and assuming we already know that
glycolysis is the process that breaks down glucose into 2 pyruvate molecules, producing 2 ATP and 2 NADH in the cytoplasm
Acetyl-CoA is a key metabolic intermediate involved in the Krebs cycle and fatty acid synthesis.
We could create new flashcards related to what we know:
Q: Where does the Krebs cycle occur?
A: In the mitochondrial matrix.
Q: How does glycolysis connect to the Krebs cycle?
A: The pyruvate from glycolysis is converted to Acetyl-CoA, which enters the Krebs cycle in the mitochondria.
Q: What is the main function of the Krebs cycle?
A: It oxidizes Acetyl-CoA to produce NADH, FADH₂, and a small amount of ATP, linking glycolysis to the electron transport chain.
Q: What does oxygen combine within the electron transport chain?
A: Electrons and protons. (This combination forms water during cellular respiration).
Q: What role does oxygen play in cellular respiration?
A: Oxygen acts as the final electron acceptor in the electron transport chain (by combining electrons and protons to form water).
Q: What is the primary function of the electron transport chain (ETC)?
A: The ETC uses electrons from NADH and FADH₂ to pump protons across the inner mitochondrial membrane, creating a gradient that drives ATP synthesis (via oxidative phosphorylation).
Takeaway:
Imagine we have no clue what Acetyl-CoA and glycolysis are, the very same flashcards will be harder to recall, leading to lower performance. A solution is using extra information in parenthesis like “(via oxidative phosphorylation)” as it is not required to answer that question, but we will have another flashcard whose purpose will be recalling how, that is, via oxidative phosphorylation.
Final Thoughts
Spaced repetition systems hold tremendous promise for learning new languages—or any subject—but they aren’t a magic bullet. The key is to be aware of these pitfalls:
Manage Flashcard Overload: Set daily review limits and prioritize high-yield cards.
Be Patient: Understand that the real benefits of SRS appear over the long term.
Craft Thoughtful Flashcards: Whether using pre-made or self-made content, focus on clear, active recall-driven prompts.
Stay Consistent: Regular, daily practice is far more beneficial than irregular, crammed sessions.
Build Connections: Ensure your flashcards form a cohesive network of knowledge rather than isolated trivia.
By keeping these challenges in mind, you can tailor your SRS experience to work with your demanding schedule and learning style. Embrace the process, refine your approach over time, and remember that persistence is the key to transforming SRS into one of your most valuable study tools.
Schmidmaier R, Ebersbach R, Schiller M, Hege I, Holzer M, Fischer MR. Using electronic flashcards to promote learning in medical students: retesting versus restudying. Med Educ. 2011 Nov;45(11):1101–10.
Kornell N, Bjork RA. Learning Concepts and Categories. Psychol Sci. 2008 Jun 1;19(6):585–92.
Rawson KA, Dunlosky J. Optimizing schedules of retrieval practice for durable and efficient learning: How much is enough? J Exp Psychol Gen. 2011 Aug;140:283–302.
Kang SHK. Spaced Repetition Promotes Efficient and Effective Learning. Policy Insights Behav Brain Sci. 2016 Mar 13;3(1):12–9.
Karpicke JD, Roediger HL. The Critical Importance of Retrieval for Learning. Science (1979). 2008 Feb 15;319(5865):966–8.
Bjork RA, Bjork EL. A new theory of disuse and an old theory of stimulus fluctuation. In: Essays in honor of William K Estes, Vol 1: From learning theory to connectionist theory; Vol 2: From learning processes to cognitive processes. Hillsdale, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc; 1992. p. 35–67.
Karpicke JD, Roediger HL. The Critical Importance of Retrieval for Learning. Science (1979). 2008 Feb 15;319(5865):966–8.
Cepeda NJ, Pashler H, Vul E, Wixted JT, Rohrer D. Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis. Psychol Bull. 2006;132(3):354–80.
Deci EL, Ryan RM. Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior. Boston, MA: Springer US; 1985.
Baumeister RF, Bratslavsky E, Muraven M, Tice DM. Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;74(5):1252–65.
Karpicke JD, Blunt JR. Retrieval Practice Produces More Learning than Elaborative Studying with Concept Mapping. Science (1979). 2011 Feb 11;331(6018):772–5.
Sweller J. Cognitive Load During Problem Solving: Effects on Learning. Cogn Sci. 1988 Apr 11;12(2):257–85.
Paivio A. Mental Representations. Oxford University Press; 1990.
Bartlett FC, Kintsch W. Remembering. Cambridge University Press; 1995.
Brod G, Werkle-Bergner M, Shing YL. The influence of prior knowledge on memory: a developmental cognitive neuroscience perspective. Front Behav Neurosci. 2013 Oct;7:139.
Piaget J. The Psychology of Intelligence. Routledge; 1950.
Vygotsky LS. Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Harvard University Press; 1978.
Brown JS, Collins A, Duguid P. Situated Cognition and the Culture of Learning. Educational Researcher. 1989 Jan 1;18(1):32–42.
Schmitt N. Review article: Instructed second language vocabulary learning. Language Teaching Research. 2008 Jul 1;12(3):329–63.
Nation ISP. Learning Vocabulary in Another Language. Cambridge University Press; 2001.
What are your thoughts on this article?Share your feedback